JOSPT Special
Issue
June, 2019
If you
are interested in Lumbopelvic Motor Control it’s a great resource that synthesizes
past literature, reflects on where we have been, and where we might look to the
future. This issue presents several clinical commentaries. I have chosen one of
interest and the following blog references key points and provides reflections
on my own practice.
Motor Control Changes in Low Back Pain: Divergence in Presentations
and Mechanisms
JH Van Dieen, PhD, NP Reeves, PhD, G Kawchuk, PhD, LR
Van Dillen, PT, PhD, PW Hodges PT, PhD, DSc MedDr, BPhty (Hons). JOSPT. 2019;(49)6:370-379.
I think
most of us can agree, from our understanding of the theory and literature on
motor control (Propositional Knowledge), from our clinical experience
(Experiential Knowledge), and from our own personal experiences (Personal
Knowledge)1,2, that individuals with low back pain (LBP) move
differently than those without LBP. These 3 types of knowledge, described by
Higgs and Titchen, guide our clinical decision making and shape our clinical
pattern development. The interesting point of this current article and one that
has not been so explicit in my own mind, is that it brings to light that we
don’t really understand the why behind these changes in motor control in
patients with LBP……..yet!
The
neuromuscular system has a variety of strategies to carry out particular tasks,
and having options is thought to keep our system balanced and healthy. Yet, this
variability may also be one reason why there are so many different
presentations of impairments related to LBP, thus making it difficult to match
the correct intervention. Which brings us to the need we have as a profession
to be able to categorize patients with LBP.
The
identification of subcategories of LBP is how we organize and make sense of varying
presentations in order to provide the most relevant intervention. I have better
outcomes when I utilize categorization systems, such as the Movement System
Impairment Model, Mechanical Diagnosis and Therapy (McKenzie), Motor Control Training, the
Integrated Systems Model (Diane Lee and the concept of regional
interdependence)3, and Janda’s Upper and Lower Crossed, and Layered Syndromes4,5.
In this
clinical commentary by Dieen et al., the authors present an interesting new thought
in this arena, one that may provide an overarching perspective on motor control changes in the
presentation of LBP, though there is much work to be done to develop and
validate this concept. This concept describes two possible phenotypes, either a Tight Control System or a Loose Control System. More on this later,
let’s take a brief look at the evidence that is currently out there.
When considering motor control of the trunk in
individuals with and without LBP, two areas have been commonly studied; trunk muscle
activation patterns and trunk movements.
Changes in trunk muscle activation and muscle
morphology have been observed in patients with LBP compared to those without
LBP. Muscle
activity has been shown to increase, decrease, demonstrate no change, activate early,
and/or activate late. I think
you will see some familiar patterns presented here.
Muscle
|
Motor control changes with LBP
|
Lumbar extensors -
|
↑ or ↓ or no change
|
Transverse abdominis (TrA) and lumbar multifidus (LM)
-
|
late
activation
|
Abdominal muscles -
|
no change and early activation of oblique muscles
|
Muscle activity -
|
delayed
onset or delayed offset with different intensities of recruitment
|
Multifidus -
|
muscle fiber changes Type I to Type II,
muscle atrophy, fatty infiltration
*indirectly
changing lumbopelvic motor control
|
Dieen et al., offer possible reasons for the
variability found in the literature and include the following: variable
strategies to accomplish tasks, differing environments in which tasks are
performed, the type of muscles recruited, as well as methodological differences
such as how data were collected through intramuscular or surface
electromyography (EMG).
The
literature surrounding alignment, posture, and trunk movement presents differences
in patients with LBP compared to those without LBP. Do you recognize these
findings?
Changes in alignment, posture, or movement with LBP
|
|
↑ lumbar
flexion
|
↑ postural sway, with or without balance impairments
|
↑posterior
pelvic tilt
|
Slower
trunk movements with dynamic tasks
|
↑ lumbar
extension
|
↓ or ↑ variability of trunk movements with bending and/or gait
|
↑
flattening of lumbar spine
|
↑ coupling of pelvis/thorax
with gait
|
No changes in alignment
|
↓ precision of control of trunk posture, movement, and
force production
|
Like muscle activation patterns, changes in
trunk alignment, posture and movement are different in individuals with LBP compared
to those without LBP and studies also demonstrate inconsistent findings. Another issue to consider when facing the inconsistency
between study results is whether motor control variability is due to intraindividual
responses (within one individual) and/or interindividual responses (between
individuals), thus adding another layer of complexity when analyzing study
outcomes. In addition, differences in study methodology may limit study
comparisons and small sample sizes may affect the validity of study outcomes.
In summary, Dieen et al. state changes in motor control are
evident with LBP, are not consistent, are not seen in all patients, and in the
end such changes may not be explained by a single factor. As a means to help sort out some of these
different responses, the authors highlight two concepts that have been
previously considered; identify the underlying mechanism for motor control
changes (for example is it a strategy in response to pain/injury or a
consequence of pain/injury) and identify the particular mechanical consequences
of such changes.
The inconsistencies and
multi-factorial nature of the findings in the literature can be confusing and
potentially overwhelming. In addition to summarizing existing rationale to help
explain the differing results in motor control changes seen in patients with LBP,
Dieen et al. present a new concept described as “tight control and loose control
phenotypes” that may help to explain the diverging responses in the literature.
These phenotypes are presented on a continuum, and could explain the two ends
of the spectrum as well as the middle ground where little or no changes in
motor control are seen (see
diagram below).
Let’s
take a closer look at the two proposed phenotypes. Both may have purposeful
short-term effects, but may also have more detrimental effects in the long run.
The
Tight Control Phenotype
In this scenario, muscle activation increases
resulting in trunk stiffening, decreased trunk movement, increased control, and
less variability of movement (or need for finely tuned anticipatory trunk
reactions). This could present as a slight change in local muscle activation,
to co-contraction of multiple muscles, or to a complete avoidance of a
particular task. In the short term, this may “protect” the system but over the
long run this may lead to increased spinal loading and less options for
movement. In essence, these individuals avoid excessive movements.
The
Loose Control Phenotype
In this scenario, muscle activation decreases
resulting in reduced tissue loading from the larger muscles that may exert
compressive forces on the spine, less control of trunk posture and movement with
potentially larger variations in movement. This reduced control of the spine at
both mid and end ranges, creates the potential for increased tissue strains and
pain. In essence, these individuals avoid excessive muscle forces.
……..so, both scenarios can lead
to abnormal tissue loading around the spine but due to different mechanisms,
thus it makes sense that intervention approaches would need to be different to
address the system impairments at
hand.
Also,
consider that these phenotypes could be present in a system at the same time,
so a mixture of approaches may be needed.
Considerations
for intervention approaches
We
could start by asking if a tight or loose control phenotype is at play and is
it in the early stages after an injury or in the later stages with more potential
for negative impacts on the system?
Tight control phenotype
If your
patient/client is avoiding movement, moving en bloc, or has a stiff quality to
their movements consider a tight control system. Goals may be to reduce
excitability and co-contraction, or “dial down overactive muscles”:
· Utilize Janda’s theory of tonic
muscles4 (phylogenetically older muscles8) to identify
possible effects of a tight control phenotype
· Assess and retrain breathing
patterns
· Post-isometric relaxation4
to overactive muscles
· Facilitate the antagonists of
the overactive muscles
· Unloading techniques for the
spine:
o
PT
places hands on lateral sides of rib cage and lifts upward to unload the lumbar
spine (in patients with a “compressed lumbar spine” this can relieve symptoms)
o
Supine
with pillow under knees or hooklying
o
Quadruped7
o
Modified
quadruped7 – standing and leaning on table with weight bearing
through forearms
o
Lumbar
self-traction in sitting (hands on arm rests and straighten elbows allowing
trunk to hang with gravity)
· Increase movement gradually, in
non-threatening ways
· Encourage variability of
movement and movement in multiple planes
· Graded exposure to avoided
activities
Loose control phenotype
If your
patient/client demonstrates excessive movements or aberrant movement patterns
such as excessive lumbo-pelvic rotation, hinging or shearing at a specific
segment consider a loose control system. Goals may be to increase excitability,
or “facilitate underactive muscles”:
· Utilize Janda’s theory of phasic
muscles4 (phylogenetically younger muscles8) to identify
possible effects of a loose control phenotype
· Identify and retrain aberrant
movement patterns
· Identify the weak link in the
chain and retrain that muscle or group of muscles
· Facilitate inhibited or weak
muscles in the chain4
· Post-isometric relaxation to the
antagonist of the inhibited or weak muscle(s)
Therefore,
intervention approaches may be completely opposite depending on the phenotype
that is presented or dominant.
As
participants in the Movement Links Seminars and certification courses, physiotherapists
have experience in looking for these divergent patterns. Through a structured
exam, we look to identify aberrant movement patterns, muscle imbalances
(muscles that need inhibition and muscles that need facilitation), the cause(s)
as well as the source of symptoms, and develop an understanding of the
relationship between impairments that are key to understanding the patient’s
movement dysfunction. The end result of such a specific exam is to be able to
identify specific movement impairments and their contributing factors and then
provide specific interventions.
Dieen et al. state that the concept of these two
phenotypes need further development and validation before they can be translated into
guidelines. This development process needs to include validation of assessments
related to these two phenotypes that can identify specific patterns to be treated,
provide targeted intervention approaches, with the goal of improved outcomes.
As such, it could be an interesting time for clinicians to write case reports utilizing
tight and loose control concepts, possibly offering some support for or insight
into directions for future research studies encompassing these two phenotypes.
All of
you awesome clinicians out there, this is where you shine – grab a resource for
writing case reports and give it a shot!
Tracey Wagner is an Assistant Professor at Samuel Merritt University teaching musculoskeletal courses and is working on developing a part-time clinical practice and her line of scholarship in the area of movement science. She is a DNS Certified Practitioner, as well as a Movement Links Certified Clinician and Instructor. She moved to the beautiful Bay Area after a 17-year career working for Kaiser Permanente Woodland Hills, serving there as a clinical specialist, mentor, and educator. Tracey has been published in the Journal of Orthopaedic and Sports Physical Therapy (JOSPT) for a case report on muscle imbalances of a professional triathlete and received the California Physical Therapy Association’s 2010 Clinician Research Publication Award.
---
References
- Higgs J, Titchen A. Propositional,
professional and personal knowledge in clinical reasoning. In: Higgs J,
Jones M: Clinical reasoning in the health professions. Oxford:
Butterworth-Heinemann; 1995.
- Higgs J, Titchen A, Neville V. Professional practice and knowledge. In: Higgs J, Titchen A, eds. Practice Knowledge & Expertise in the Health Professions. Oxford: Butterworth Heinemann; 2001:3-9.
- Hides, JA, Donelson R, Lee D, Prather H, Sahrmann SA, Hodges PW. Convergence and Divergence of Exercise-Based Approaches That Incorporate Motor Control for the Management of Low Back Pain. J Orthop Sports Phys Ther. 2019;49(6):437-452.
- Page, P, Frank, C, Lardner R. Assessment and Treatment of Muscle Imbalance The Janda Approach. Human Kinetics 2010.
- Morris CE, Greenman PE, Bullock MI, Basmajian JV, Kobesova A. Vladimir Janda, MD, DSc: tribute to a master of rehabilitation. Spine. 2006;31(9):1060-1064.
- JH Van Dieen, PhD, NP Reeves, PhD, G Kawchuk, PhD, LR Van Dillen, PT, PhD, PW Hodges PT, PhD, DSc MedDr, BPhty (Hons). Motor Control Changes in Low Back Pain: Divergence in Presentations and Mechanisms. JOSPT. 2019;(49)6:370-379.
- Sahrmann SA. Diagnosis and Treatment of Movement Impairment Syndromes. St. Louis MO: Mosby Inc. 2002.
- Kolar P et al. Clinical Rehabilitation. 1st ed. Prague, Czech Republic: Alena Kobesova, Rehabilitation Prague School;2013.
No comments:
Post a Comment